Lek mating

From Infogalactic: the planetary knowledge core
(Redirected from Lek paradox)
Jump to: navigation, search

<templatestyles src="Module:Hatnote/styles.css"></templatestyles>

Greater sage-grouse at lek, with multiple males displaying for the less conspicuous females

A lek is an aggregation of males that gather to engage in competitive displays that may entice visiting females who are surveying prospective partners for copulation.[1] Leks are commonly formed before or during the breeding season. A lekking species is defined by the following characteristics: male displays, strong female mate choice, and the conferring of male indirect benefits.[2] Although lekking is most prevalent among avian species, lekking behavior is found in insects, amphibians, and mammals as well.

Etymology

Lek mating arena, modeled on the sage grouse, in which each male, alpha-male (highest ranking), beta-male, gamma-male, etc., guards a territory of a few meters in size on average, and in which the dominant males may each attract up to eight or more females.[3] In addition, each individual is shown with variations in personal space (bubbles), whereby higher-ranking individuals have larger personal space bubbles.[4] Common bird leks typically have 25–30 individuals. A strict hierarchy accords the most desirable top-ranking males the most prestigious central territory, with ungraded and lesser aspirants ranged outside. Females come to these arenas to choose mates when the males' hierarchy has become established, and preferentially mate with the dominants in the centre.

The term derives from the Swedish lek, a noun which typically denotes pleasurable and less rule-bound games and activities ("play", as by children). English use of lek dates to the 1860s. Llewelyn Lloyd's The Game birds and wild fowl of Sweden and Norway (1867) introduces it (capitalised and in single quotes, as 'Lek') explicitly as a Swedish term.[5]

Lekking species

The term was originally used most commonly for black grouse ("orrlek") and for capercaillie ("tjäderlek"), and lekking behavior is quite common in birds of this type, such as sage grouse, prairie chicken, great bustard and sharp-tailed grouse. However, it is also shown by birds of other families, such as the ruff, great snipe, Guianan cock-of-the-rock, musk ducks, hermit hummingbirds, manakins, birds-of-paradise, screaming pihas and the kakapo. Lekking is also seen in some mammals such as the Ugandan kob (a waterbuck), some pinnipeds, several species of fruit bat, and the topi antelope. Lekking is also seen in marine iguanas[6] and some species of fish (e.g., Atlantic cod, desert pupfish,[7] and the cichlid Astatotilapia burtoni). Even insects like the midge and the ghost moth demonstrate lekking behavior. Lekking is also found in some paper wasp species such as Polistes dominula,[8] the orchid bee Eulaema meriana, [9] in some butterfly species like the black swallowtail (Papilio polyxenes),[10] and in tarantula hawks like Hemipepsis ustulata.

A posing capercaillie

Lekking behaviour

There are two types of lekking arrangements: classical and exploded. In the classic lekking system, male territories are in visual and auditory range of their neighbours. In an exploded lek, males are further away from one another than they would be in a classic lek. Males in an exploded lek are outside of visual range of one another, but they stay within earshot.[11] Exploded lek territories are much more expansive than classic systems and are known to exhibit more variation.[12] A famous example of exploded leks is the "booming" call of the kakapo, the males of which position themselves many kilometres apart from one another to signal to potential mates.[13]

A group of three male great-tailed grackles that may or may not attract the attention of a receptive female

Lek territories of different taxa are stable and do not vary in terms of size and location.[14] Males often return to the same mating sites because of female fidelity.[15] It has been shown that avian females such as the black grouse, and great snipe are faithful to males and not mating sites.[16] Successful males congregate in the same area as the previous breeding season because it is familiar to them, while females return to reunite with said males. It has been observed that they will not return to a mating site if their male partner is not present.[16] Another possible explanation for lek stability may result from male hierarchies within a lek. In manakins, subordinate betas may inherit an alpha’s display site, increasing the chances of female visitation.[16] Rank may also contribute to the stability of lek size, as lower ranking males may congregate to achieve a perceived optimal size as a way to attract females.[17]

Some species of ants, such as red harvester ants, as well as certain bee species, like tetragonisca angustula and trigona spinipes exhibit lek-like mating patterns.[18][19] Males form reproductive aggregations, congregating and collectively give off a pheromone that attracts reproductive females. The more males present to give off the pheromone, the stronger the attraction for the females.

Lek-mating is also relatively common among the paper wasp species. For example, Polistes dominula males often fight with other males in mid-air to appeal their superiority and attractiveness. Males who lose will fly away from the lek. Females fly through leks or perch near lekking areas to observe males before making choices on mates and they use the highly conspicuous abdominal spots on males, which are highly variable in size and shape, to aid in mate choice. Males with smaller, more elliptically shaped spots are more dominant over other males and preferred by females compared to males who have larger, more irregularly shaped spots.[20] In comparison, Mischocyttarus flavitarsis males will choose a perch site near female hibernation areas, rub their abdomens to mark their territory and wait 6–7 weeks for a female to approach. If an intruder approaches, the owner of site will lunge and grapple the other wasp. Typically, they will fall off the perch site and finish the fight on the ground until one of them wins.[21]

Costs and benefits

Lekking behavior in the Clusiid fly Paraclusia tigrina

The main benefit for both sexes is mating success. For males, the costs stem from females’ preferences. The traits that are selected for, may be energetically costly to maintain and may cause increased predation. For example, increased vocalization rate caused a decrease in the mass of male great snipe.[22] Another cost would be male competition, as females prefer victorious males. Great snipes regularly fight to display dominance or defend their territory.[22] Aggressive male black grouse are preferred over non-aggressive males and when the males fight they tear feathers from each other's tails.[23] At first glance, it would seem that females receive no direct benefits because these males are only contributing genes to the offspring.[24] However, lekking actually reduces the cost of female searching because the congregating of males makes mate selection easier.[25] Females do not have to travel as far, since they are able to evaluate and compare multiple males within the same vicinity. This may also help reduce the amount of time a female may be vulnerable to predators. Female Hyperolius marmoratus under predatory pressure consistently chose leks near their release sites, and high male calling rates reduced female search time.[26]

Female mating preferences

A meta analysis of 27 species found that qualities such as lekking size, male display rate, and the rate of male aggression exhibit positive correlation with male success rates.[1] A positive correlation was also found between attendance, magnitude of exaggerated traits, age, frequency of fights, and mating success.[1] This female preference leads to mating skew, with some males being more successful at copulating with females. The variation in mating success is quite large in lek mating systems with 70-80 percent of matings being attributed to only 10%-20% of the males present.[27]

The lek paradox

Persistent female choice for particular male trait values should erode genetic diversity in male traits and thereby remove the benefits of choice, yet choice persists.[28] The enigma of how additive genetic variation is maintained in the face of consistent female preference is named the “lek paradox”. This paradox can be somewhat alleviated by the occurrence of mutations introducing potential differences, as well as the possibility that traits of interest have more or less favorable recessive alleles.

The basis of the lek paradox is continuous genetic variation in spite of strong female preference for certain traits. There are two conditions in which the lek paradox arises, the first is that males contribute only genes and the second is that female preference does not affect fecundity.[29] Female selection should lead to directional selection, which would result in a greater prevalence for that trait. Stronger selection should lead to impaired survival because there will be a decrease in genetic variance since more offspring will have similar traits, which is also known as “runaway selection”.[30] Lekking species do not exhibit runaway selection.

In a lekking reproductive system, what male sexual characteristics can signal to females is limited, as the males provide no resources to females or parental care to their offspring.[31] This implies that females gain indirect benefits from her choice in the form of “good genes” for her offspring.[32] Hypothetically, in choosing a male who excels at courtship displays, females will gain genes for her offspring that will increase their survival or reproductive fitness.

Zahavi declared that male sexual characteristics only convey useful information to the females if these traits confer a handicap on the male.[33] Otherwise, males could simply cheat: if the courtship displays have a neutral effect on survival, males could all perform equally and it would signify nothing to the females. But if the courtship display is somehow deleterious to the male’s survival—such as increased predator risk or time and energy expenditure—it becomes a test by which females can assess male quality. Under the “handicap principle”, males who excel at the courtship displays prove that they are of better quality and genotype, as they have already withstood the costs to having these traits.[33] Resolutions have been formed to explain why strong female mate choice does not lead to runaway selection. The handicap principle describes how costly male ornaments provide females with information about the male’s inheritable fitness.[34] The handicap principle may be a resolution to the lek paradox, for if females select for the condition of male ornaments, then their offspring will have better fitness.

One potential resolution to the lek paradox is Rowe and Houle’s theory of condition-dependent expression of male sexually selected traits. Similar to the handicap principle, Rowe and Houle argue that sexually selected traits depend on physical condition. Condition, in turn, summarizes a large number of genetic loci, including those involved in metabolism, muscular mass, nutrition, etc. Rowe and Houle claim that condition dependence maintains genetic variation in the face of persistent female choice, as the male trait is correlated with abundant genetic variation in condition.[32] This is also called the "genic capture" hypothesis, which describes how a significant amount of the genome is involved in shaping the traits that are sexually selected.[31] There are two criteria in the genic capture hypothesis: the first is that sexually selected traits are dependent upon condition and the second is that general condition is attributable to high genetic variance.[35]

Genetic variation in condition-dependent traits may be further maintained through mutations and environmental effects. Genotypes may be more effective in developing condition dependent sexual characteristics in different environments, while mutations may be deleterious in one environment and advantageous in another.[31] Thus genetic variance remains in populations through gene flow across environments or generation overlap. According to the genic capture hypothesis, female selection does not deplete the genetic variance, as sexual selection operates on condition dependence traits, thereby accumulating genetic variance within the selected for trait.[35] Therefore, females are actually selecting for high genetic variance.

In an alternate but non-exclusionary hypothesis, Hamilton and Zuk proposed that successful development of sexually selected traits signal resistance to parasites.[36] Parasites can significantly stress their hosts so that they are unable to develop sexually selected traits as well as healthy males. According to this theory, a male who vigorously displays demonstrates that he has parasite-resistant genes to the females. In support of this theory, Hamilton and Zuk found that male sexual ornaments were significantly correlated with levels of incidence of six blood diseases in North American passerine bird species. The Hamilton and Zuk model addresses the lek paradox, arguing that the cycles of co-adaptation between host and parasite resist a stable equilibrium point. Hosts continue to evolve resistance to parasites and parasites continue to bypass resistant mechanisms, continuously generating genetic variation.[36] The genic capture and parasite resistance hypotheses could logically co-occur in the same population.

One resolution to the lek paradox involves female preferences and how preference alone will not cause a drastic enough directional selection to diminish the genetic variance in fitness.[37] Another conclusion is that the preferred trait is not naturally selected for or against and the trait is maintained because it implies increased attractiveness to the male.[29] Thus, it may be possible that there is no paradox.

Evolution

Hotshot hypothesis

There have been several hypotheses proposed as to why males cluster into leks. The hotshot hypothesis is the only model that attributes males as the driving force behind aggregation. The hotshot model hypothesizes that attractive males, known as the “hotshots”, garner both female and male attention.[38] Females go to the hotshots because they are attracted to these males. Other males form leks around these hotshots as a way to lure females away from the hotshot. A manipulative experiment using the little bustard, Tetrax tetrax, was done to test the various lek evolution models.[39] The experiment involved varying the size and sex ratio of leks using decoys. To test whether or not the presence of a hotshot determined lek formation, a hotshot little bustard decoy was placed within a lek. After the fake hotshot was added to the lek, both male and female visitation to the lek increased.[39]

Hotspot model

The hotspot model considers the female density to be the catalyst for the clustering of males. This model predicts that leks will form where females tend to reside as a way to increase female interaction.[40] Female manakin traffic has been observed to be concentrated around leks, bathing sites, and fruiting areas, with males aggregated near the most visited fruiting resources.[40] The hotspot model also predicts that lek size is dependent upon the amount of females inhabiting a patch of land.[38] To test if the number of females affects lek formation, a group of female little bustard decoys were added to a lek. The presence of these female decoys did not have an effect on lek size.[39]

Blackhole model

The blackhole model proposes that females have a preference for neither size nor type of male, but rather that females tend to be mobile and will mate wherever leks may be located.[39] This model predicts that female mobility is a response to male harassment.[41] This prediction is difficult to test, but there was a negative correlation found between male aggressiveness and female visitation in the little bustard population.[39] Evidence supporting the black hole model is mainly found in ungulates.[15]

Kin selection

An alternative hypothesis for lekking is kin selection, which assumes that males within a lek are related to one another. As females rarely mate outside of leks, it is advantageous for males to form leks.[42] Although not all males within a lek will mate with a female, the unmated males still receive fitness benefits. Kin selection explains that related males congregate to form leks, as a way to attract females and increase inclusive fitness.[16] In some species, the males at the leks show a high-degree of relatedness, but this does not apply as a rule to lek-forming species in general.[43][44][45] In a few species such as peacocks and the black grouse, leks are composed of brothers and half-brothers. The lower-ranking males gain some fitness benefit by passing their genes on through attracting mates for their brothers (larger leks attract more females). Peacocks recognize and will lek with their brothers, even if they have never met before.[46]

Predation Protection

Another hypothesis is predator protection, or the idea that there is a reduction in individual predation risk in a larger group.[39] This could work both for the males in within the group as well as any female who visits the lek.[47] Protection also explains the presence of mixed leks, when a male of one species joins a lek of another species for protection from a common set of predators. This occurs with manakins,[48] as well as other birds such as grouse species.[49]

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 Fiske, P., Rintamaki, P. T. & Karvonen, E. Mating success in lekking males: a meta-analysis. Behavioral Ecology 9, 328–338 (1998).
  2. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  3. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  4. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  5. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.. Lloyd also loans 'Lek-ställe' (Swedish lekställe) for "pairing ground".
  6. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found. open access publication - free to read
  7. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  8. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  9. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  10. Lederhouse, Robert C. (1982). "Territorial Defense and Lek Behavior of the Black Swallowtail Butterfly, Papilio polyxenes". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 10 (2): 109–118.
  11. Trail, P. W. "Why Should Lek-Breeders be Monomorphic?" Evolution 44, 1837–1852 (1990).
  12. Jiguet, F., Arroyo, B. & Bretagnolle, V. Lek mating systems: a case study in the Little Bustard Tetrax tetrax. Behavioural Processes 51, 63–82 (2000).
  13. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  14. Durães, R., Loiselle, B. a, Parker, P. G. & Blake, J. G. "Female mate choice across spatial scales: influence of lek and male attributes on mating success of blue-crowned manakins". Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 276, 1875–1881 (2009).
  15. 15.0 15.1 Isvaran, K. Variation in male mating behaviour within ungulate populations: patterns and processes. Current Science, 89(7), 1192-1199(2005).
  16. 16.0 16.1 16.2 16.3 Duval, E. H. "Female mate fidelity in a lek mating system and its implications for the evolution of cooperative lekking behavior". The American Naturalist 181, 213–22 (2013).
  17. Hernandez, M. L., Houston, A. I. & Mcnamara, J. M. "Male rank and optimal lek size". Behavioral Ecology 10, 73–79 (1999).
  18. H.W. Velthuis, Hayo; Koedam, Dirk; L. Imperatriz-Fonseca, Vera (2005-01-01). "The males of Melipona and other stingless bees, and their mothers". Apidologie 36 (2): 169–185.
  19. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  20. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  21. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  22. 22.0 22.1 Hoglund, J., Kalais, J. A. & Fiske, P. "The costs of secondary sexual characters in the lekking great snipe (Gallinago media)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 30, 309–315 (1992).
  23. Alatalo, R. V, Höglund, J. & Lundberg, a Lekking in the black grouse: a test of male viability. Nature 352, 155–156 (1991).
  24. Reynolds, J. D. & Gross, M. R. "Costs and Benefits of Female Mate Choice : Is There a Lek Paradox ?" 136, 230–243 (1990).
  25. Wickman, P. & Jansson, P. "An estimate of female mate searching costs in the lekking butterfly Coenonympha pamphilus". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 40, 321–328 (1997).
  26. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  27. Mackenzie, A., Reynolds, J. D., V.J, B. & Sutherland, W. "Variation in Male Mating Success on Leks". The American Naturalist 145, 633–552 (1995).
  28. Miller, Christine and Allen Moore. (2007). "A potential resolution to the lek paradox through indirect genetic effects". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274:1279-1286
  29. 29.0 29.1 Kirkpatrick, M. Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Female Choice. Evolution 36, 1–12 (1982).
  30. Kirkpatrick, M. & Ryan, M. "The evolution of mating preferences and the paradox of the lek". Nature 350, 33–38 (1991).
  31. 31.0 31.1 31.2 Tomkins, Joseph L. "Genic capture and resolving the lek paradox". TRENDS in Ecology and Evolution. Vol.19 No.6 June 2004.
  32. 32.0 32.1 Rowe, Locke and David Houle. (1996). "The lek paradox and the capture of genetic variance by condition dependent traits". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 263:1415-1421
  33. 33.0 33.1 Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—a selection for a handicap." Journal of Theoretical Biology 53:205-214.
  34. Iwasa, Y., Pomiankowski, A. & Nee, S. "The Evolution of Costly Mate Preferences II: The ’Handicap' Principle". Evolution 45, 1431–1442 (1991).
  35. 35.0 35.1 Rowe, L. & Houle, D. "The Lek Paradox and the Capture of Genetic Variance by Condition Dependent Traits". Proceedings: Biological Sciences 236, 1415–1421 (1996).
  36. 36.0 36.1 Hamilton, W. D. and M. Zuk. (1982). "Heritable true fitness and bright birds: A role for parasites?". Science 218:384-387.
  37. Pomiankowski, a & Moller, a P. "A Resolution of the Lek Paradox". Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 260, 21–29 (1995).
  38. 38.0 38.1 Foster, M. S. & Beehler, B. M. "Hotshots, Hotspots, and Female Preferences in the Organization of Lek Mating Systems". The American Naturalist 131, 203–219 (1998).
  39. 39.0 39.1 39.2 39.3 39.4 39.5 "Jiguet, F. & Bretagnolle, V. "Manipulating lek size and composition using decoys: An experimental investigation of lek evolution models". The American Naturalist 168, 758–768 (2006).
  40. 40.0 40.1 Théry, M. "The evolution of leks through female choice: Differential clustering and space utilization in six sympatric manakins". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 30, 227–237 (1992).
  41. Clutton-Brock, T. H., Price, O. F. & Maccou, A. D. C. "Mate retention, harassment, and the evolution of ungulate leks". Behavioral Ecology 3, 234–242 (1991).
  42. Durães, R., Loiselle, B. a & Blake, J. G. "Spatial and temporal dynamics at manakin leks: Reconciling lek traditionality with male turnover". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62, 1947–1957 (2008).
  43. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  44. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  45. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  46. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  47. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  48. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.
  49. Lua error in package.lua at line 80: module 'strict' not found.

Further reading

External links